Melatonin has a major physiological function in mammals: the transmission of information about daylength for the organisation of daylength dependent seasonal functions (4). Evidently the importance in a given species depends to the extent on which that species is daylength dependent. Humans are not markedly seasonal but nevertheless they show evidence of residual photoperiodic effects in a number of ways.(5). Some powerful seasonal effects in sub human mammals include stimulation or inhibition of the reproductive system and there is certainly evidence for reproductive effects (largely inhibitory) in humans (4).
There is however very little evidence for an important function of melatonin in adult humans even though in the neonatal period it may be capable of a long term influence on circadian rhythmicity (6). A possible physiological role for melatonin in the organisation and timing of human sleep is suggested by a number of lines of correlative evidence (7). Possibly the most convincing are observations of sleep in blind subjects. Many visually impaired subjects and especially those with no light perception at all show 'free running' circadian rhythms of sleep, core temperature melatonin and cortisol secretion (8,9,10). Thus depending on individual periodicity the peak of melatonin secretion occurs intermittently during the daytime when it is strongly associated with a tendency to 'nap'. Similarly in abnormally synchronised subjects, a daytime peak of melatonin is associated with sleepiness or sleep (11).
Free running sighted individuals show strong coupling between 'sleep propensity' and the melatonin peak, and sleep deprived subjects show coupling between 'maximum fatigue' and the melatonin peak (12). However melatonin is clearly not essential for the maintenance of circadian rhythms in a normal environment. Many humans take beta blockers which suppress the melatonin rhythm (13) but, whilst there are some reports of sleep disturbance this is not apparently a major problem. There is reason to believe that if the pineal or melatonin are absent, compensatory systems must operate. If however melatonin is present but with an abnormal rhythm it may cause problems.
Can melatonin synchronise human circadian rhythms?
Recently in collaboration with the MoD/DERA we have had the opportunity to investigate the effects of daily melatonin administration in normal sighted human volunteers free running in continuous very dim light (31). Ten normal healthy men were maintained in identical conditions in two separate groups in partial temporal isolation under constant dim light (<8lux) for two periods of 30 days.with attenuated sound and ambient temperature variations. Importantly the subjects had access to a digital clock recording real time. Knowledge of clock time is essential to timed drug administration during free run so that subjects are not disturbed by experimenters entering the unit at specified treatment times.
In these circumstances the majority of individuals free run with a mean period (tau) of 24.3h (39). This tau is substantially shorter than the average tau for human circadian rhythms reported by Wever and colleagues (40), but is consistent with recent data from Campbell et al, and Czeisler et al.(41, 42) In a double blind, randomised cross-over design subjects received 5mg melatonin at 2000h for the first 15 days (melatonin 1st) followed by placebo for the second 15 days (placebo 2nd) or vice versa (placebo 1st, melatonin 2nd) during leg 1 with treatment being reversed in leg 2. The variables measured were melatonin (as 6-sulphatoxymelatonin), rectal temperature (every 6 minutes by rectal probe), activity and sleep (actigraphy and logs). Temperature was demasked mathematically.
Nine out of ten subjects showed free running rhythms in all variables (i.e. tau was significantly different from 24h by cosinor and regression analysis) when placebo was given for the first 15 days, whilst melatonin for the first 15 days stabilised the sleep wake cycle to 24h in eight of ten individuals. Two subjects showed highly irregular sleep with melatonin first and throughout the period of melatonin administration. Sleep consolidated rapidly on changing to placebo administration. In some subjects there was a shortening of the period of the temperature rhythm without synchronisation.
Melatonin, taken after a 15 day period of free run under placebo (melatonin 2nd) induced phase advances in five subjects, phase delays in two subjects and stabilisation of the sleep wake cycle in two subjects, (one subject was lost to the trial during this leg). Subsequently synchronisation of the sleep wake cycle to 24h was found in the majority (7/9) of individuals. Temperature continued clearly to free run in four subjects.
The obvious explanation for these heterogeneous results was the fact that melatonin was given at different circadian times in each subject depending on their individual free running period. Thus we calculated the time of administration relative to core temperature maximum. Maximum phase advances in core temperature were seen when the first melatonin treatment was given approximately 2 hours after the temperature acrophase and phase delays were seen with administration times 5-6 hours before core temperature maximum. These data, plotted as a conventional PRC are similar to the PRCs reported by Lewy et al and Claustrat et al (29, 30) using endogenous melatonin as a phase marker, but with a higher amplitude (31).
It is important to note however that whilst phase shifts in temperature were present, apparent synchronisation was not consistent. Moreover where the periodicity of temperature was clearly free running, the tau was shorter than that found with placebo 1st. We consider it is likely that even when apparent synchronisation of temperature was seen that the effect may well have been a shortening of tau to a period indistinguishable from 24h in this short time series. Long term studies of a blind man taking 5 mg melatonin daily have also shown synchronisation of sleep with shortening of temperature tau (43). Long term studies of a free running sighted man receiving daily melatonin in a normal environment also suggest shortening of tau rather than synchronisation (44). To what extent synchronisation of sleep is a masking effect i.e. immediate sleepines induction, rather than a circadian effect on the timing mechanisms of sleep remains an open question.
The two subjects who showed highly irregular sleep with melatonin as a first treatment raise some interesting and potentially very important questions. The initial melatonin treatment was given closer to core temperature maximum in these subjects compared to the majority of the others.
We wished to confirm this observation and performed a second shorter experiment with six subjects maintained in identical conditions. They completed a three day baseline in a normal light dark cycle (dark/sleep 2300-0700) and then took melatonin at 2000h for six days again as a double\blind crossover against placebo. These conditions were devised to time administration to within two hours of core temperature maximum. Again two subjects showed highly irregular sleep. Thus of 16 subjects taking melatonin in continuous dim light, 4 have shown this effect (45).
These observations clearly do not fit a simple model of melatonin action and evidently they need to be confirmed in a larger number of subjects. There may be an analogy with the “squashing” of the amplitude of T and cortisol and large phase jumps of the circadian system induced by bright light given at or very close to T min in a constant routine (46). Conceivably melatonin may provoke a similar response if suitably timed with respect to the sleep wake cycle. Another possible explanation may involve interactions of the circadian and homeostatic components of sleep whereby a complex combination of sleep induction by out of phase melatonin goes some way towards compensating sleep debt and a subsequent bout of sleep occurs due to the circadian component of sleep (Process C, 47) which itself may then be shifted by the melatonin administration.
However if melatonin is considered to be primarily a photoperiodic signal yet other possible interpretations are suggested. In most species melatonin secretion is related to the length of the night: the longer the night the longer the duration of secretion (4). If humans are kept strictly in darkness for 14 hours per day for a period of two months, the melatonin secretion pattern expands to cover almost the entire dark period and concomitantly, in extended periods of 16 hours of light, the rhythm contracts. In these conditions sleep is consolidated into two primary blocks with a period of wakefulness between the episodes (5). This is consistent with animal data. The sleep wake cycle is not always a consolidated block in very long or very short days or in constant conditions in animals. The phenomenon of splitting occurs (48).
Effectively sleep occurs in two major episodes. Underlying this phenomenon are two theoretical oscillators, the evening (E) and morning (M), or dawn and dusk oscillators. In constant conditions they may uncouple spontaneously and run with different periods. In changing daylength they may remain coupled but as dawn occurs later and dusk occurs earlier with decreasing daylength they move apart and with increasing daylength they move together. The phase relationship will determine whether one or two episodes of sleep occur. There is reason to believe that the evening onset of melatonin is associated with the E oscillator and that the morning decline is associated with the M oscillator (49). Moreover in sufficiently long nights melatonin also shows evidence of splitting. Two peaks are seen in both sheep and humans, in the latter case extended periods of 16h darkness per day are necessary to show this clearly although two (and even three) peaks are not unknown in a normal environment (50, 51).
The fragmentation of sleep with melatonin treatment may represent spontaneous uncoupling of 2 (or more) oscillators, although it did not occur in the same subjects in the same conditions with placebo administration or in the same subjects in the same conditions with different circadian treatment times. Melatonin given close to temperature maximum affects the E oscillator (marked by onset of melatonin secretion) more than the M oscillator (marked by offset) (26, 27) and in consequence we consider that the timing of treatment may have induced such a large phase advance of E that it became dissociated from M.
The effect if confirmed may be dose related as well as circadian time related in that maximum phase advancing treatment may increase the likelihood of fragmentation. Whatever the explanation it is clear that this phenomenon would be highly undesirable if consolidated sleep and alertness is required. Moreover if human sleep is indeed controlled by two or more oscillators, theories of sleep regulation (e.g. the two process model) may have to be revised. Finally a strategy of splitting sleep into two components with melatonin may lead to more rapid adaptation to phase shift by advance of the E and delay of the M component.
These observations are important in view of the widespread self medication with melatonin in the USA, the substantial interest in melatonin for adapting to phase shift and optimising sleep and performance, and the potential therapeutic use of melatonin in various sleep and other disorders. They require confirmation and evaluation. Whatever the explanation it is clear that melatonin does not act as a conventional sleep inducing drug.
Back to the top.
References
1. Arendt J. Melatonin. Brit Med J 1996;312:1242-1243.
2. Bonn D. Melatonin's multifarious marvels: miracle or myth? Lancet 1996; 347:184
3. Sugden D. Psychopharmacological effects of melatonin in mouse and rat. J Pharmacol Exper Therap 1983; 227:587-591.
4. Arendt J. Melatonin and the mammalian pineal gland. Chapman Hall, London, 1994.
5. Wehr TA. The durations of human melatonin secretion and sleep respond to changes in daylength (photoperiod). J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1991; 73:1276-1280.
6. Davis FC, Mannion J. Entrainment of hamster pup circadian rhythms by prenatal melatonin injections. Am J Physiol 1988; 255:R439-R448.
7. Arendt J (1997) Melatonin. In:: Schwartz W, ed. Sleep Science: Integrating Basic Research and Clinical Practice, Karger Monographs in Clinical Neuroscience, Vol 15, Karger, Basel, New York.
8. Miles LEM, Wilson MA. High incidence of cyclic sleep/wake disorders in the blind. Sleep Res 1977; 6:192.
9. Lewy AJ, Newsome DA: Different types of melatonin circadian secretory rhythms in some blind subjects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1983; 56:1103-1107.
10. Lockley SW, Skene DJ, Arendt J, Tabandeh H, Bird A, Defrance R. Relationship between melatonin rhythms and visual loss in the blind. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1997; 82: 3763-3770.
11. Lockley SW, Skene DJ, Tabandeh H, Bird AC, Defrance R, Arendt J. Relationship between napping and melatonin in the blind. J Biol Rhythms 1997; 12:16-25
12. Akerstedt T, Gillberg M, Wetterberg I. The circadian covariation of fatigue and urinary melatonin. Biol Psychiatry 1982; 17: 547-554.
13. Arendt J, Bojkowski C, Franey C, Wright J, et al. Immunoassay of 6-hydroxymelatonin sulfate in human plasma and urine: abolition of the urinary 24-hour rhythm with atenolol. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1985; 60, 1166-1173.
14. Lerner AB, Case JD, Takahashi Y, et al. Isolation of melatonin, pineal factor that lightens melanocytes. J Amer Chem Soc 1958; 80, 2587.
15. Lerner AB, Nordlund JJ. Melatonin: Clinical Pharmacology. J Neural Transm 1978; Suppl. 13: 339-347.
16. Cramer H, Rudolph J, Consbruch V. On the effects of melatonin on sleep and behaviour in man. In : Costa E, Gessa GL, Sandler M, eds, Serotonin-New vistas: Biochemistry and behavioural and Clinical Studies. Advances in Biochemical Psychopharmacology, Volume 11, Raven Press, New York, 1974, 187-191.
17. Arendt J, Borbely AA, Franey C, Wright J. The effects of chronic small doses of melatonin given in the late afternoon on fatigue in man: a preliminary study. Neurosci Lett 1984; 45: 317-321
18 Tzichinsky O, Lavie P. Melatonin possesses time-dependent hypnotic effects. Sleep 1994 ; 7:638-45
19. Dijk DJ, Roth C, Landolt H.-P et al. Melatonin effect on daytime sleep in men: suppression of EEG low frequency activity and enhancement of spindle frequency activity. Neurosci Lett 1995; 201: 13-16.
20. Dollins AB, Zhdanova IV, Wurtman RJ et al. Effect of inducing nocturnal serum melatonin concentrations in daytime on sleep, mood, body temperature, and performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1994; 91:1824-1828.
21. Garfinkel D, Laudon M, Nof D, Zisapel N. Improvement of sleep quality in elderly people by controlled-release melatonin. Lancet. 1995; 346: 541-544.
22. Singer C, Sack B, Lewy AJ. Melatonin treatment of the elderly. Associated Professional Sleep Societies Conference, June 10-15th, 1997, San Francisco.
23. Cagnacci A, Elliott JA, Yen SSC Melatonin: a major regulator of the circadian rhythm of core temperature in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1992; 75, 447-452
24. Deacon S, Arendt J. Melatonin induced temperature suppression and its acute phase-shifting effects correlate in a dose dependent manner in humans. Brain Res 1995; 688: 77-85.
25. Cajochen C, Kraeuchi K, Moeri D, Hetsch C, Wirz-Justice A. A single administration of melatonin or the melatonin agonist S-20098 lengthens the first REM sleep episode. Sleep Res 1995, 24: 40.
26. Arendt J, Bojkowski C, Folkard S et al. Some effects of melatonin and the control of its secretion in humans. In : Evered D, Clark S, eds, Photoperiodism, Melatonin and the Pineal Gland , Ciba Foundation Symposium 117, Pitman, London, 1985: 266-283.
27. Wright J, Aldhous M, Franey C, English J, Arendt J. The effects of exogenous melatonin on endocrine function in man. Clin Endocrinol 1986; 24: 375-382.
28. Gillette MU, McArthur AJ. Circadian actions of melatonin at the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Behav Brain Res 1996; 73 135-139
29. Lewy AJ, Ahmed S, Latham Jackson JM, Sack RL. Melatonin shifts human circadian rhythms according to a phase response curve. Chronobiol Int 1992; 9: 380-392.
30. Zaidan R, Geoffriau M, Brun J, Taillard J, Bureau C, Chazot G, Claustrat B. Melatonin is able to influence its secretion in humans - description of a phase response curve. Neuroendocrinol 1994; 60: 105-112.
31. Middleton B, Stone B, Arendt J. Complex effects of melatonin on human circadian rhythms in constant dim light. J Biol Rhythms 1997; 12:467-477.
32. Arendt J, Aldhous M, Marks V. Alleviation of jet-lag by melatonin: preliminary results of controlled double-blind trial. Brit Med J 1986; 292: 1170.
33. Arendt J, Aldhous M, Marks M. et al. Some effects of jet-lag and their treatment by melatonin. Ergonomics 1987; 30: 1379-1393.
34. Arendt J, Deacon S. Treatment of circadian rhythm disorders: melatonin. Chronobiol Internat 1997;14:185-204.
35. Arendt J, Aldhous M, Wright J. Sychronisation of a disturbed sleep-wake cycle in a blind man by melatonin treatment. Lancet 1988; 1: 772-773.
36. Sack RL, Lewy AJ, Blood ML et al. Melatonin administration to blind people: phase advances and entrainment. J Biol Rhythms 1991; 6: 249-261.
37. Folkard S, Arendt J, Aldhous M, Kennett H. Melatonin stabilises sleep onset time in a blind man without entrainment of cortisol or temperature rhythms. Neurosci Lett 1990; 1133: 193-198.
38. Aldhous ME, Arendt,J. Assessment of melatonin rhythms and the sleep wake cycle in blind subjects. In:: Arendt J, Pevet P, eds, Proceedings of the European Pineal Society, Guildford,1990.Adv Pineal Res 1991; 5: 307-311.
39. Middleton B, Arendt J, Stone BM. Human circadian rhythms in constant dim light (8 lux) with knowledge of clock time. J Sleep Res 1996; 5: 69-76.
40. Wever RA. The circadian system of man.: results of experiments under temporal isolation. New York, Springer Verlag, Heidelberg, 1979.
41. Campbell SS, Dawson D, Zulley J. When the human circadian system is caught napping: evidence for endogenous rhythms close to 24 hours.Sleep 1993; 16: 638-40.
42. Czeisler CA, Duffy JF, Shanahan TL, et al.Reassessment of the intrinsic period (t) of the human circadian pacemaker in young and older subjects. Abstract 505, Second International Congress of the World Federation of Sleep Research Societies, Nassau, The Bahamas, 12-16th September, 1995.
43. Arendt J, Lockley SW, Skene DJ, unpublished results.
44. McArthur AJ, Sack RL, Hughes RJ, Lewy AJ. Melatonin, a stronger zeitgeber than light in some sighted individuals? Sleep Res 1995; 24A: 527
45. Middleton B, Stone BM, Arendt J. Melatonin and fragmented sleep patterns. Lancet 1996; 348: 551-552.
46. Jewett ME, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA. Light induced circadian suppression of endogenous circadian amplitude. Nature 1991; 350: 59-62.
47. Borbely AA. A two-process model of sleep regulation, Human Neurobiology 1982; 1: 195-204.
48. Pittendrigh CS, Daan S. A functional analysis of circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents. V. Pacemaker structure: a clock for all seasons. J Comp Physiol 1976; 106: 333-355.
49. Illnerova H, Vanecek J. Complex control of the circadian rhythm in N-acetyltransferase activity in the rat pineal gland, In: Aschoff J, Daan S, Groos G, eds, Vertebrate Circadian Systems, Springer Verlag, Heidelberg, Berlin, 1982: 285-296.
50. Arendt J, Symons AM, Laud C. Pineal function in the sheep: evidence for a possible mechanism mediating seasonal reproductive activity. Experientia 1981; 37: 584-589.
51. Arendt J. Mammalian pineal rhythms. Pineal Res Rev 1985; 3:161-213.
52. Deacon S, Arendt J. Adapting to phase-shifts, I. An experimental model for jet lag and shift work. Physiol Behav 1996; 59, 665-673.
53. Deacon S Arendt J. Adapting to phase-shifts, II. Effects of melatonin and conflicting light treatment. Physiol Behav 1996; 59, 675-682.
Back to the top.
|